Category Archives: Bird Behavior

Brazen In Winter

Northern mockingbird blocks a northern cardinal (photo by Marge Van Tassel)

This week I wrote about northern mockingbirds having knock-down drag-out fights during the breeding season. Did you know that mockingbirds are also territorial in the winter? Even though they’ve migrated far from their breeding territory they vigorously defend their wintering zone.

Last winter Marge Van Tassel had a pushy mockingbird in her Armstrong County yard. She sent this photo saying, “The male of the pair in our yard chased all birds away from the food bowl last year when he was in it. He even chased this female cardinal back to a bush.”

Yikes! She’s putting on the brakes with her tail.

Mockingbirds are brazen, even in winter.

(photo by Marge Van Tassel)

The More The Merrier

Eurasian tree sparrow, great tit and blue tit at a feeder in Bulgaria (photo from Wikimedia Commons)

Squirrels eventually figure out a puzzling bird feeder, but have you ever noticed that birds do too? Lone songbirds usually can’t do it, but scientists have proven that a flock can. The bigger the flock the better.

I learned this years ago when I set up a “goldfinch-only” thistle feeder whose holes forced birds to perch upside down to eat the seeds. This is easy for American goldfinches but not for the birds I meant to foil — house sparrows. Lone house sparrows gave up on this feeder but the flock tried a lot of methods. A few of them would land right-side up and slowly fall forward until they were upside down. One of them put out his wing to grab the feeder. The rest of the flock followed.

On Throw Back Thursday, learn how songbird flocks can solve puzzles faster than individuals in this vintage article: Two Heads Are Better Than One.

The more birds at our feeders the merrier we are … and so are they.

(photo from Wikimedia Commons; click on the caption to see the original)

Knock Down, Drag Out: Mockingbirds Fight

  • Northern Mockingbirds fighting #1 (photo by Beth Signorini)

In case you think that songbirds are innocent, meek creatures consider the northern mockingbird (Mimus polyglottus). He always has “attitude” and he’s willing to engage in a knock down drag out fight.

Beth Signorini captured these two during the breeding season.

I wonder who won.

(photos by Beth Signorini)

Murmurations in Lorain

Murmuration of starlings in Lorain, Ohio, 30 Dec 2018 (video by Chad+Chris Saladin)

European starlings (Sturnus vulgaris) are famous for their ability to fly in tight formation. When under attack by a peregrine falcon, they evade him in amazing ways.

Starlings under pressure fly closer together and shape-shift the flock like a giant blob in the sky. This makes it hard for the peregrine to choose a single bird as prey and gives their flocks a special name, a murmuration.

This winter Chad+Chris Saladin have been filming murmurations in Lorain, Ohio. Above on 30 December 2018, below on Christmas Day.

European starling murmuration in Lorain, Ohio on Christmas Day 2018 (video by Chad+Chris Saladin)

Whenever there’s a peregrine, the starlings murmur.

Bonus! Here’s a Facebook album by Chad+Chris with closeups of a peregrine hunting starlings. (click on the “See More” link embedded in the Facebook post)

(videos and photos by Chad+Chris Saladin; click on the captions to visit their Facebook page)

Casting A Pellet

(Red-tailed hawk casts a pellet, photos by Chad+Chris Saladin)

Why does this red-tailed hawk throw up a long gray lump? Is he sick? Not at all. He’s casting a pellet.

Birds’ digestive systems are very different from ours, beginning with their beaks. Since birds don’t have teeth they swallow most of their food whole. The rest of their digestive system is geared to deal with this.

Birds have little saliva and few taste buds compared with mammals, which chew and physically process food as the first step and then subject it to chemical processing as the second step. Birds reverse this sequence. They start chemical digestion in the proventriculus [then the food] undergoes physical processing in the gizzard.

Ornithology, 3rd Edition by Frank B. Gill, page 164

We chew with our teeth and spit out the bones. Birds chew with their gizzards which then collect the bones, fur, and other indigestible bits into a lump. The bird spits out the lump when it’s a convenient size.

Owls, eagles, hawks and falcons cast pellets but so do many other birds “including grebes, herons, cormorants, gulls, terns, kingfishers, crows, jays, dippers, shrikes, swallows, and most shorebirds.” (quote from Wikipedia)

Scientists examine pellets to find out what the bird ate. One of the long-eared owl pellets below was dissected to reveal the rodent bones inside.

Pellets cast by a long-eared owl, dissected to show rodent bones (photo from Wikimedia Commons)

For whatever reason, it’s rare to see a bird casting a pellet so consider yourself lucky if you witness it, as Chad+Chris Saladin did in the photos above.

A NOTE ABOUT HANDLING OWL PELLETS from Wikipedia: Some rodent viruses and bacteria can survive the owls’ digestive system so wear gloves and sterilize the pellets in a microwave oven before handling. A 2005 study found outbreaks of salmonellosis at elementary schools associated with dissection of owl pellets: Smith KE, Anderson F, Medus C, Leano F, Adams J, 2005. Vector-Borne and Zoonotic Diseases,5, 133–136.

(red-tailed hawk photos by Chad+Chris Saladin; pellet photo from Wikimedia Commons; click on the captions to see the originals)

Chasing Crows

Unimpressed by the University of Pittsburgh’s scarecrow recordings, Pittsburgh’s winter crow flock continues to roost on campus. On Saturday evening, 29 December 2018, Claire Staples and I counted them for the Christmas Bird Count. It was harder than it sounds.

We began at 4pm at the highest vantage point we could find — the top deck of a parking garage between Trees Hall and the Petersen Events Center. From there we could see two huge streams of crows arriving from the south and east, pouring into the trees near Kennard Playground. Unfortunately the playground is over the edge of the hill so those crows went out of sight as soon as they landed.

By 5pm there were so many crows that new arrivals were landing on Trees Hall roof. We were pretty confident we’d counted 10,000 … and then they began to leave. What?!

The crows were headed for Schenley Heights so we followed in the car. We thought they might roost in the Heights but they were restless and on the move. Those in the air were headed for the Cathedral of Learning. We chased them down the hill.

Sure enough, Tennyson Avenue between Alumni and Clapp Halls was “Crow Central.” It was impossible to count them in the air as you can see in Claire’s video.

But I had another way to count them. Since 2016 I’ve tallied Crows-per-tree and Trees-with-crows to get an estimate of the Pitt roost. By car we tallied 45 trees with crows. With an average of 230 crows per tree that’s 10,350 crows at Pitt. Close enough. 10,000.

We think this is only half the crows in Pittsburgh. Claire and I saw just two of the four streams of crows that head to town at dusk from the north, south, east and west.

Where do the crows from the north and west spend the night? Are there 10,000 of them, too? We didn’t have time to chase them.

A clue concerning the origin of the Pitt crow roost

On Saturday Claire and I reminisced about the 2012 CBC when she and three other women counted 20,000 crows from a vantage point in the Strip District. That year a large part of the roost used trees at the closed housing project, Addison Terrace, that was downhill from Kennard Playground. In the spring of 2013 Addison Terrace was demolished and redeveloped into new homes. The first time Pitt had real trouble with crows was in the fall of 2013 when Addison Terrace was no longer available as a roost. Hmmmm!

(video by Claire Staples, 29 Dec 2018)

Crows, Do Not Enter!

Jungle crow in Japan (photo from Wikimedia Commons)

In 2015 the International Coastal Research Center in Otsuhi, Japan figured out how to keep crows from pillaging their headquarters building.  With the advice of a crow expert they posted “Crows Do Not Enter” signs and the crows stayed away!  Here’s how it happened.

On 11 March 2011 the town where ICRC is located, Otsuchi, Japan, was devastated by nearly 30 foot waves from the Great East Japan Earthquake and tsunami. The photo below of the Kirikiri section shows how Otsuchi was wiped out.

Aerial view of tsunami damage in Kirikiri area of Otsuchi, Japan. 3 months later, 20 Jun 2011 (photo from Wikimedia Commons)

The International Coastal Research Center headquarters was severed damaged as well (the three-story building in the photo below).

Tsunami damage at Otsuchi, Japan, 4 days later 15 Mar 2011 (photo from U.S. Navy rescue operations via Wikimedia Commons)

The first two floors were wrecked, as seen in two photos from the ICRC website.  (Click here to see more photos of the damage.)

Tsunami damage to ICRC (images via ICRC Recovery website. Click this caption to see the originals in context)

However, by 2015 the local jungle crows (Corvus macrorhynchos) could not resist stealing the damaged materials inside the building.  During the nesting season they flew into the open building, ripped insulation off the pipes and carried it to their nests.  

To get rid of the crows the ICRC asked for help from Tsutomu Takeda, an environmental scientist and crow expert.  Rather than using scarecrow tactics he hung large signs on the pipes, visible from outdoors, that said “Crows Do Not Enter.”  

“Crows, Do Not Enter” signs at ICRC (photo from an ICRC article which is no longer online)

As soon as the signs went up the crows stayed away. This method was still working two years later when Mother Nature Network published the news.

Can the crows read the signs? No, but people can and when they do they look up to see if crows are in the building.  The crows hate it when people watch them stealing nesting material so they stopped doing it.

If the signs worked on jungle crows in Japan, perhaps they’ll work on American crows, too.  I wonder if our winter crow flock would stop roosting at the University of Pittsburgh if they put “Crows Do Not Enter” signs in the trees.  😉

(This post was inspired by MNN: Wild Crows seem to obey Do Not Enter signs, and a report from the ICRC which is no longer online.  Photos are from Wikimedia Commons, the ICRC Recovery Project website and the now-missing ICRC article. I encourage you to click on the captions to see the originals.)

Racing Pigeons And Raptors

Pigeons (Columba livia) and the raptors who hunt them have evolved together for millions of years. The raptors’ successful hunts leave only the fastest, most maneuverable pigeons. Speedy, elusive pigeons mean only the most skillful raptors can survive.  Most of us never get to see this interaction so this dramatic video from Romania is a real treat.

In 9 minutes Porumbeiro shows how his racing pigeons work to elude two raptors: first a peregrine falcon (Falco peregrinus), then a northern goshawk (Accipiter gentilis). 

The pigeons stay in a tight flock because raptors can’t pick out a victim in a moving ball of birds. The raptors try to separate one bird from the group by slicing through the flock. If it works, the raptor pursues the lone bird.

Who will win?

(video by pomumbeiro on YouTube)

The Lookout Bird

Dusky-throated antshrike at banding station (photo courtesy of Cameron Rutt)

When you’re vulnerable to predators it pays to stick together and have a good lookout to warn you of danger.

The dusky-throated antshrike (Thamnomanes ardesiacus) doesn’t look important but he’s quick to notice the presence of hawks and falcons and has a distinctive alarm call that wakes up the forest to impending danger.  It turns out that he’s key to the foraging location and cohesion of his mixed species flocks in the Amazon.

Early this year, a study by San Francisco State University temporarily removed dusky-throated antshrikes from their mixed species flocks in Peru. They discovered that within hours the flocks left their semi-open mid-story locations for denser parts of the forest.  Often the flocks without an antshrike completely dissolved.

What does the alarm call sound like?  Is it loud? Does it grab your attention? You bet!  Here’s the sound of a worried dusky-throated antshrike:

His role in the flock works so well that the same mix of species sticks together for generations. As San Francisco State Professor Vance Vredenburg remarked, “You come back to the same habitat after 20 years, and the same flocks are using the same areas of the forest.”

Read more about the study here in Science Daily.

p.s. When the antshrike is not afraid he sings this buzzy, rising song. (audio by Peter Boesman at Xeno Canto #271766)

Dusky-throated antshrike song (Xeno Canto 271766)

(media credits: photo of dusky-throated antshrike by Cameron Rutt, audio alarm call from Macaulay Library, audio song from Xeno Canto; click on the captions to see the originals)